Jump to content

Conflict (process)

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
(Redirected from Conflict (sociology))
Jean Rogers, John Wayne, and Ward Bond in the film Conflict (1936)

A conflict is a situation in which inacceptable differences in interests, expectations, values, and opinions occur in or between individuals or groups.

Definitions

[edit]

Depending on the source, there are different definitions for conflicts:

  • Disagreements, discrepancies, and frictions that occur when the actions or beliefs of one or more members of the group are unacceptable to one or more other group members and are rejected by them.[1]
  • An interaction between actors (individuals, groups, organizations, etc.), where at least one actor experiences incompatibilities in thinking/imagination/perception and/or feeling and/or wanting with the other actor (the other actors) in such a way that in realizing an impairment by another actor (the other actors) occurs.[2]
  • Contradictory interests that are represented by different people or groups of people and who are dependent on each other in achieving their interests (or at least believe this).[3]
  • Interactive processes that manifest themselves in incompatibility, disagreement, or dissonance within or between social entities.[4]
  • A state of tension that arises because there are irreconcilable contradictions between two or more parties with regard to a certain good.[5]
  • Activities that take place when conscious beings (individuals or groups) want to take actions that do not match their desires, needs, or obligations.[6]
  • Situations where hostile behavior occurs.[7]
  • Behavior that deliberately hinders the achievement of another's goals.[8]
  • State of objective incompatibility between values or goals.[9]

Main types of conflict

[edit]
School children fighting in Jamaica

In cases of intragroup conflict, there is a conflict between the overall goals of the general group, and the goals of at least one person in that group.[10] The disagreements may also be examples of interpersonal conflict, a conflict between two or more people.[11] Intrapersonal conflicts are conflicts occurring in an individual, for example a bad conscience or an identity conflict. Intergroup conflict is conflict between two or more groups.[11]

Conflict types

[edit]

More specific types of conflict include the following.

  • Content conflict occurs when individuals disagree about how to deal with a certain issue. This can be a good thing as it has the potential to stimulate discussion and increase motivation.[12]
  • Relationship conflict occurs when individuals disagree about one another. This relational conflicts decreases performance, loyalty, satisfaction and commitment, and causes individuals to be irritable, negative and suspicious.[12] This stems from interpersonal incompatibilities. It is an awareness of frictions caused by frustrations, annoyance, and irritations. Relationship conflict is comparable to affective and cognitive conflict as defined by Amason and Pinkley, respectively.[13]
  • Process conflict refers to disagreement over the group's approach to the task, its methods, and its group process.[13] They note that although relationship conflict and process conflict are harmful, task conflict is found to be beneficial since it encourages diversity of opinions, although care should be taken so it does not develop into a process or relationship conflict.[13]
  • Task conflict is related to disagreements in viewpoints and opinion about a particular task in group settings. It is associated with two interrelated and beneficial effects. The first is group decision quality. Task conflict encourages greater cognitive understanding of the issue being discussed. This leads to better decision making for the groups that use task conflict.[13] The second is affective acceptance of group decisions. Task conflict can lead to increased satisfaction with the group decision and a desire to stay in the group.[14]
  • Affective conflict is an emotional conflict developed from interpersonal incompatibilities and disputes. It often produces suspicion, distrust, and hostility. Therefore, it is seen as a negative kind of conflict and an obstacle to those who experience it and is described as "dysfunctional."[15]
  • Cognitive conflict occurs during tasks and comes from a difference in perspective and judgement. It improves decision making and allows for the freer exchange of information between group members. Cognitive conflict is seen as a positive tension that promotes good group work.[15]

The following are examples of conflict that could be either intragroup or intergroup conflict.

Geographical categorization

[edit]

Conflicts can also be categorized geographically, as in the North-South conflict and the East-West conflict. Other examples are territorial conflicts such as the Kosovo War, the Iraq-Iran War, the Middle East conflict, the China-Taiwan conflict and the Korean Conflict.

People categorization

[edit]

Likewise, conflicts can be categorised according to the people involved. Areas in which conflicts frequently occur are, for example, in the family, between parents, between siblings or between parents and children, among friends and acquaintances, in groups, in school, in nature, in business between companies, employers or employees,[22] in science,[23] between generations (generational conflict), between ethnic groups (ethnic conflict) or within or between states (see peace research).

Course of conflict

[edit]

Conflicts are not static events in themselves, but develop a dynamic that can become uncontrollable in extreme cases. The course of a conflict can be divided into four phases:[24]

Phase of conflict Name Description
Phase I Latent phase The conflict situation emerges
Phase II Conscience Both parties recognise the conflict
Phase III Action Both parties react based on their thoughts and emotions
Phase IV Interactions Both parties interact with alternating conflict modes

Conflict escalation

[edit]

Beyond that a conflict can further escalate. Models of escalation in conflicts are the Friedrich Glasl's model of conflict escalation,[2] the conflict curve by Michael S. Lund[25][26][27] and the hourglass model by Oliver Ramsbotham.[25][28] When an escalation is initiated by one party there often is a sequence of escalation behaviour: requests, demands, angry remarks, threats, harassment and abuse.[29] Models with a fixed order of stages of conflict escalation have been criticized for not representing the probabilistic nature of conflicts.[30]

Conflict behavior

[edit]
Hierarchy of arguments according to their escalation potential by Graham

The various ways in which people react to conflict situations have been presented by Gerhard Schwarz, sometimes in reference to Eric Lippmann, as follows:[31][32]

  • Flight (Evasion, Avoidance)
  • Fight (Enforcement, Destruction)
  • Subordination (Adaptation, Concession, Unilateral Acceptance)
  • Delegation (of the problem to another instance)
  • Compromise (Agreement with advantages and disadvantages on both sides)
  • Consensus (Cooperation)

These behavior patterns can lead to the solution or dissolution of a conflict in different situations. While the first-mentioned stages are anti- or confrontational in character, the last-mentioned stages represent forms of constructive conflict resolution - with consensus as the highest (to be learned) form.[32][33] Paul Graham divided forms of argument hierarchically according to their escalation potential and the quality of the argument.[34] In the ‘‘dual concern model’’, conflict types are divided along the two dimensions: orientation towards one's own goal or orientation towards the goal of the conflict partner.[1][35] Personality tests for conflict behavior are the Kraybill Conflict Style Inventory,[36] the open-source licensed "Ethics Position Questionnaire"[37] and the Thomas-Kilmann Conflict Mode Instrument. Further, more general and comprehensive personality tests are Leadership Derailers,[38] Social value orientation,[39][40] Hexaco-PI-R[41] and NEO-PI-R,[42] which also include a bit of conflict behaviour.

Conflicts between individuals

[edit]

Often, further phenomena occur in the course of a conflict. Doubts and uncertainties about one's own position are usually replaced with firm convictions (confirmation bias), without anything having changed in the real probabilities.[43][1] Moreover, people often stick to their conviction in order not to lose face, even if the conviction is now being questioned by themselves.[44] The reciprocity (“an eye for an eye”) favors a conflict escalation[45] and a convergence of behavior when the other side consistently shows competitive or consistently collaborative behavior to achieve their goals.[1] However, collaborative behavior tips more easily into competitive behavior than vice versa.[1] In conflicts, destructive behaviours can also appear: violence, coercion, intimidation, blackmailing, deception und seduction.[46]

Negative emotions such as anger and fear make it difficult to work through the differences.[47][1] Moreover, anger is often contagious, because a person who is met with anger reacts in turn more often angrily.[48] Likewise, behaviors such as hostile, overly aggressive, choleric, conflict-avoiding, evasive, passive-aggressive, nagging or accusing (without changing anything), non-changing, annoying, pessimistic, superior or indecisive behavior can make conflict resolution difficult.[49]

The attribution of presumed strengths, attitudes, and values to the other conflict party is often distorted during a conflict.[50][1] Likewise, there is often a false attribution of whether the problem arises from the situation or the character of the participants.[51] As a conflict escalates, the tactics used by each side become more confrontational (harder). However, there are situations where the threatened party fares better when a threat is not met with a counter-threat.[52][53][1] A too dominant negotiation style can provoke a blocking stance as a reaction.[54][55][1] In conflicts between equally strong parties, a competitive conflict style is avoided if a strong counter-reaction is expected.[56][1] While at the beginning of a conflict escalation a counter-reaction tends to be disproportionately high, it is rather disproportionately low at a higher level of escalation.[1] Often at the beginning of a conflict escalation, various coalitions are formed to support one's own interests, which later in the conflict lead to conflicts between two groups.[1]

Conflicts between groups

[edit]

When a conflict occurs not just between two individuals (interpersonal conflict), but between two or more groups (intergroup conflict), additional effects of group dynamics come into play.[57][58] Five typical emotions have been identified in groups that contribute to escalation: superiority, injustice, vulnerability, mistrust, and helplessness.[59] Additionally, envy, contempt, pity and admiration can also occur between groups. Envy results when the out-group is perceived to have high competence, but low warmth.[60][61] Envious groups are usually jealous of another group's symbolic and tangible achievements and view that group as competition.[58] Contempt results when the out-group is taken to be low in both competence and warmth.[60][61] According to Forsyth, contempt is one of the most frequent intergroup emotions. In this situation, the out-group is held responsible for its own failures. In-group members also believe that their conflict with the out-group can never be resolved.[58] Out-groups that are believed by the in-group to be high in warmth but low in competence are pitied.[60][61] Usually pitied groups are lower in status than the in-group and are not believed to be responsible for their failures.[58] Admiration occurs when an out-group is taken to be high in both warmth and competence, however, admiration is very rare because these two conditions are seldom met.[60][61] An admired out-group is thought to be completely deserving of its accomplishments. Admiration is thought to be most likely to arise when a member of the in-group can take pride in the accomplishments of the out-group, and when the out-group achieving does not interfere with the in-group.[58]

Groups often exhibit more competitive behavior than individuals within a group do with each other.[58] Merely perceiving one's own group identity already favors discrimination against foreign groups.[58] When individuals with a collaborative conflict style join a group, a switch to a competitive group conflict style (group behavior) can occur.[58] Additionally, other effects of dominant behavior within the group and between groups come into play.[58] Motivations such as greed, fear, and social identity increase in groups.[58] If the potential reward for greed is reduced, the effect of greed diminishes.[58] There is a double standard that manifests itself primarily in an enhancement of the actions of one's own group, but also in a devaluation of the actions of other groups.[58] This also includes distorted generalizations and stereotypes attributed to the other group.[58] It involves both deindividuation (opponents are only perceived as part of a homogeneous group, not as individuals), and dehumanization (opponents are perceived as subhuman).[62] In an experiment, more than half of the participants opted for a choice with less reward if the process was perceived as fair in return.[58][63]

A key player in inter-group relations and conflict is the collective sentiment a person's own group (in-group) feels toward another group (out-group). These inter-group emotions are usually negative, and range in intensity from feelings of discomfort when interacting with a member of a certain other group to full on hatred for another group and its members. For example, in Fischer's organizational research at the University of Oxford, inter-group conflict was so 'heated' that it became mutually destructive and intractable, resulting in organizational collapse.[64][65]

Out-group-directed emotions can be expressed both verbally and non-verbally, and according to the stereotype content model, are dictated by two dimensions: the perceived warmth (friendliness) and competence of the other group (skillfulness). Depending on the perceived degree of warmth and competence, the stereotype content model predicts four basic emotions that could be directed toward the out-group (Forsyth, 2010).

Factors

[edit]

Although the involved parties may hope to reach a solution to their dispute quickly, psychological and interpersonal factors can frustrate their attempts to control the conflict, and in this case, conflict escalation occurs. Conflict escalation "can be understood as an intensification of a conflict with regard to the observed extent and the means used".[66] A number of factors including increased commitment to one's position, use of harder influence tactics, and formation of coalitions propel the escalation of the conflict.[67]

Uncertainty and commitment

[edit]

As conflicts escalate, group members' doubts and uncertainties are replaced with a firm commitment to their position. People rationalize their choices once they have made them: they seek out information that supports their views, reject information that disconfirms their views, and become more entrenched in their original position (also see confirmatory bias).[68] Additionally, people believe that once they commit to a position publicly, they should stick with it. Sometimes, they may realize the shortcomings of their views, but they continue defending those views and arguing against their opponents just to save face.[69] Finally, if the opponents argue too strongly, reactance may set in and group members become even more committed to the position.[70][71]

Perception and misperception

[edit]

Individuals' reactions to the conflict are shaped by their perception of the situation and people in the situation. During the conflict, opponents' inferences about each other's strengths, attitudes, values, and personal qualities tend to be largely distorted.[72]

Misattribution

[edit]

During the conflict, people explain their opponents' actions in ways that make the problem worse. Fundamental attribution error occurs when one assumes that opponents' behavior was caused by personal (dispositional) rather than situational (environmental) factors.[73] When conflict continues for a while, opponents might decide that this conflict is intractable. People usually expect intractable conflicts to be prolonged, intense, and very hard to resolve.[74]

Misperceiving motivations

[edit]

During the conflict, opponents often become mistrustful of one another wondering if their cooperative motivations were replaced by competitive ones. This loss of trust makes it difficult to return to the cooperative relationship. People with competitive social value orientations (SVOs) are the most inaccurate in their perception of opponents' motivation. They often think that others compete with them when in fact, there is no competition going on.[75] Competitors are also more biased in their search for information that confirms their suspicions that others compete with them.[76] They also tend to deliberately misrepresent their intentions, sometimes claiming to be more cooperatively oriented than they actually are.[77]

Soft tactics and hard tactics

[edit]

People use soft tactics at the outset of the conflict, but as it escalates, tactics become stronger and harder. To demonstrate this phenomenon, Mikolic, Parker, and Pruitt (1997)[78] simulated a conflict situation by creating a "birthday card factory" with study participants who were paid a small amount for each card they manufactured using paper, colored markers, and ribbons. The work went well until researchers' confederate who posed as another participant started hoarding production materials. Initially, group members tried to solve the problem with statements and requests. When these methods failed they shifted to demands and complaints, and then to threats, abuse, and anger.

Although hard tactics can overwhelm the opponent, they often intensify conflicts. Morton Deutsch and Robert Krauss (1960)[79] used trucking game experiment to demonstrate that capacity to threaten others intensifies conflict. They also showed that establishing a communication link does not always help to solve the dispute.[80] If one party threatens the other, the threatened party will sometimes fare best if it cannot respond with a counterthreat.[81][82] Equally powerful opponents, however, learn to avoid the use of power if the fear of retaliation is high.[83]

Reciprocity and upward conflict spiral

[edit]

In many cases, upward conflict spirals are sustained by the norms of reciprocity: if one group or person criticizes the other, the criticized person or group feels justified in doing the same. In conflict situations, opponents often follow the norm of rough reciprocity, i.e. they give too much (overmatching) or too little (undermatching) in return. At low levels of conflict, opponents overmatch their threats, while at high levels of conflict they undermatch their threats. Overmatching may serve as a strong warning, while undermatching may be used to send conciliatory messages.[84]

Few and many

[edit]

When conflicts erupt, group members use coalitions to shift the balance of power in their favor, and it is typical for multiparty conflicts to reduce to two-party blocks over time. Coalitions contribute to the conflict because they draw more members of the group into the affray. Individuals in coalitions work not only to ensure their own outcomes but also to worsen outcomes of non-coalition members. Those who are excluded from the coalition react with hostility and try to regain power by forming their own coalition. Thus, coalitions need to be constantly maintained through strategic bargaining and negotiation.[85]

Irritation and anger

[edit]

It is generally difficult for most people to remain calm and collected in a conflict situation. However, an increase in negative emotions (i.e. anger) only exacerbates the initial conflict. Even when group members want to discuss their positions calmly and dispassionately, once they become committed to their positions, an emotional expression often replaces logical discussion.[86] Anger is also contagious: when group member negotiates with someone who is angry, they become angry themselves.[87]

Conflict resolution

[edit]

Nicholson notes that a conflict is resolved when the inconsistency between wishes and actions of parties is resolved.[88] Negotiation is an important part of conflict resolution, and any design of a process which tries to incorporate positive conflict from the start needs to be cautious not to let it degenerate into the negative types of conflict.[89] Actual conflict resolutions range from discussions between the parties involved, such as in mediations or collective bargaining, to violent confrontations such as in interstate wars or civil wars. "Between" these are the variants of lawful or courtly clarification, which by no means have to take the form of "mud fights", but can be handled as "professional delegation" of the problem to lawyers, in order to relieve oneself from the time-consuming and strenuous clarification procedure. Many conflicts can be resolved without escalation by the parties involved. If the conflict parties do not come to a solution themselves, accompanying measures can be taken by third parties.[1]

The goal of conflict resolution is an effective and lasting solution to the conflict. This is achieved through the satisfaction of all parties involved, which ideally results in constructively working together on the problem (collaboration, cooperation).[90] In addition, a regulation of the conflict can occur through a decision by an authority,[91] e.g., by an arbitrator, a court, a parent, or a supervisor. Unprocessed conflicts generate frustration and aggression, which can result in cost, damage, and scapegoats.[58]

De-escalation

[edit]

The first step in a dispute is usually de-escalation (e.g., cessation of hostilities, reduction of open aggression). A reciprocal tit for tat strategy ("an eye for an eye") can build trust between groups in the case of mutually collaborative or mutually competitive conflict styles.[58] To facilitate a change of positions in a conflict party, face-saving bridges should be built, e.g., by discussing what has already changed since the beginning of conflict resolution or by introducing common fair behavioral norms.[49]

Escalating behavior should not be reacted to immediately, to give the person or persons time to regain emotional self-control, making them more accessible to arguments and avoiding mutual escalation.[49] Anger can be reduced by an apology, humor, a recess, common behavioral norms, greater distance (switch to online discussion), or by background information that the escalation of the other side was not intended.[1] Afterwards, the problematic behavior can be addressed in a calm manner, followed by an acknowledgment of those substantive points of the escalating person that are correct.[49] Alternatively, a feedback sandwich can be used.

In the case of avoiding behavior, more questions should be asked and more attention should be paid to the participation of these persons in the conflict resolution and to their immaterial interests (such as recognition and autonomy).[49] In the conversation, a reminder can be given for motivation that the processing of the conflict serves the satisfaction of the interests of both sides.[49]

Regulated communication

[edit]

The second step is the initiation of communication between the conflicting parties, often through mediation. Accompanying conditions are described by the Harvard Concept.[92] Alternatively, the moderation cycle according to Josef W. Seifert can be followed.[93] Furthermore, I-messages can be alternated with active listening according to Thomas Gordon[94][95] or nonviolent communication according to Marshall B. Rosenberg[96] can be used to depersonalize a discussion.

Analysis

[edit]

In the third step, the actual conflict of interest is identified and mutual understanding for the interest of the other party is developed. This requires understanding and respecting the underlying values and motivations. According to the four-sides model by Friedemann Schulz von Thun, there are two levels of information in every statement: the content level and the emotional or relationship level.[97] Both levels contain interests, the differences of which to the other conflict party should be balanced as much as possible. Then a win-win solution for the conflict can be developed together.

Conflict modes

[edit]
Conflict modes according to Thomas & Kilmann

To resolve conflicts, Thomas L. Ruble and Kenneth W. Thomas[98][99] and later Whetten and Cameron[100] investigated the possible strategies in case of conflict. The variables assertiveness and cooperativity are based on the results in the 1964 work Managerial Grid by Jane Srygley Mouton and Robert Rogers Blake.[101] The two variables deal on the one hand with the question of whether the goals or interests of the two conflict parties are achieved and on the other hand with the question of how cooperativity is maintained. Kenneth W. Thomas and Ralph H. Kilmann published a rating system in 1974.[102][103] It extends the model to include compromise-seeking behavior and quantifies five typical conflict styles (competitive, collaborating, compromise seeking, avoiding and accommodating) in questionnaires, which give different values for the personal inclination to the five typical conflict styles. A collaboration (cooperation) in principle allows both the achievement of the goals of both sides and a good handling. However, not every style leads to an acceptable result in every situation. For example, a collaboration does not work if the goals of the two conflict parties are immutable and mutually exclusive. The different styles have different advantages and disadvantages.[104] Depending on the situation, different conflict styles can be considered desirable to achieve the best results.[105]

Thomas and Kilmann distinguish five typical conflict styles:[106]

Conflict style Pros and Cons Situations
Competitive
(win-lose)
* Pursuit of own objectives
* Use of power
* Can lead to disputes
* Can cause resentments
* Emergencies requiring quick decisions
* Important and unpopular decisions
* When you are certain you are right (important matters)
* To defend against others taking advantage
Collaborative
(win-win)
* Cooperation to everyone's satisfaction
* Detailed analysis of interests
* Involves cognitive conflict
* Can be time-consuming
* When a compromise is not acceptable
* To gain support for the cause
* To maintain or improve relationships
* To unify perspectives
Compromise-seeking
(½win-½win)
* Middle ground regarding assertiveness and cooperativity
* Splitting the difference
* Neither half nor whole?
* Temporary solutions for complex conflicts
* When goals of two equally strong opponents are mutually exclusive
* Under time pressure
* Evasive strategy for collaborative or competitive
Avoiding
(lose-lose)
* Neglect of both interests
* Conflicts remain unresolved
* Delaying tactic
* Hopeless situations outside of one's control
* Unimportant situations
* To let others cool down
* When others can resolve the conflict more effectively
Accommodating
(lose-win)
* Opposite of competitive
* Self-sacrifice
* Selfless generosity
* Doormat
* When the other person's issues are much more important
* To build social capital
* When clearly inferior and losing
* When harmony is especially important
* To allow subordinates to develop

Limited resources can be distributed either according to the previous investment of time, energy and resources, according to equal shares, according to power ratios or according to need.[1] A decision can be reached using various procedures. Ideally, a consensus is worked on collaboratively (and preferably on a win-win solution) because this serves the interests of all parties involved. In situations where the interests are immovable and mutually exclusive, other procedures must be used. Common procedures in which only some of the interests of both sides are served are compromise procedures or distribution imposed by a neutral authority (as in the inquisitorial or arbitration procedure, with parents or a supervisor). Furthermore, a vote or a judicial judgment is often carried out as a competitive procedure, in which the interests of the larger group or the right-preserving side are served first. Likewise, a concession by one side can resolve the conflict if there is a willingness to do so. As a concession implies at least a partial renunciation of one's own interests with little to no compensation, a willingness to do so becomes less likely with increasing escalation.

Glasl, on the other hand, assigns six strategies for conflict management to the nine escalation stages of Friedrich Glasl's model of conflict escalation.[2]

  • Level 1-3 (hardening, polarization & debate, actions instead of words): Moderation
  • Level 3-5 (actions instead of words, concern about image & coalitions, loss of face): Process support
  • Level 4-6 (concern about image & coalitions, loss of face, threatening strategies): socio-therapeutic process support
  • Level 5-7 (loss of face, threatening strategies, limited destructive strikes): conciliation/mediation
  • Level 6-8 (threatening strategies, limited destructive strikes, fragmentation): arbitration/judicial proceedings
  • Level 7-9 (limited destructive strikes, fragmentation, together into the abyss): power intervention

According to Ramsbotham, conflicts are divided into five phases of conflict development, to which three conflict resolution strategies are assigned:[25]

  • Phase of differences: conflict transformation
  • Phase of objection: conflict transformation
  • Phase of polarization: settlement of conflicts
  • Phase of violence: settlement of conflicts
  • Phase of war: containment of conflicts

While the Thomas and Kilmann system describes five typical conflict styles, aligns the measures with the conflict styles and the situations and emphasizes collaboration as a solution, the measures in the Glasl system and the Ramsbotham system are aligned with the escalation level.

Conflict mediation

[edit]

Conflict is a social process that is exacerbated when individual members of a group take sides in the debate. Among the methods to resolve conflict is mediation of the dispute by a group member not currently involved in the dispute. More specifically, a mediator is defined as a person who attempts to resolve a conflict between two group members by intervening in this conflict. Put simply, the mediator can be thought of as a disinterested guide directs the disputants through the process of developing a solution to a disagreement.[1] For conflicts with negative interpersonal relationships on a low escalation level, relationship building can help transform the nature of the relationship and improve the communication.[29] As mediation depends on meeting together peacefully, it is more successful in conflicts with low levels of escalation where there is still a will to work on an agreement.

Although the tendency will be for group members who are uninvolved in the dispute to remain uninvolved, in some cases, the sheer intensity of the conflict may escalate to the point where mediation is unavoidable and still feasible. Third party mediation of the conflict opens avenues for communication between group members in conflict. It allows members to express their opinions and request clarification of other member's standpoints while the mediator acts as a form of protection against any shame or "loss of face" that either disputant may experience. This can be done by shedding a positive light on the reconciliation that was made during the mediation process. For instance, if it was negotiated that two cashiers will rotate the weekends they work, the mediator might point out that now each worker gets a weekend off every two weeks.[107] The mediator can also offer assistance in refining solutions and making counter-offers between members, adjusting the time and location of meetings so that they are mutually satisfying for both parties.[107] There are three major mediation approaches:[1]

  1. Inquisitorial procedure: Using this procedure, the mediator asks each of the disputants a series of questions, considers the two sets of responses, and then selects and imposes a mandatory solution on the members. The inquisitorial procedure is the least popular approach to mediation.
  2. Arbitration: Here, mediation involves the two disputants explaining their arguments to the mediator, who creates a solution based on the arguments presented. Arbitration is best for low intensity conflict, but is the most favored mediation style overall.
  3. Moot: The moot approach involves an open discussion between disputants and the mediator about the problems and potential solutions. In the moot approach, the mediator cannot impose a mandatory solution. After arbitration, a moot is the most preferred mediation style. It does not always lead to a tangible result.

In practice, conflict resolution is often interwoven with daily activities, as in organizations, workplaces and institutions. Staff and residents in a youth care setting, for instance, interweave everyday concerns (meals, lessons, breaks, meetings, or other mundane but concerted projects) with interpersonal disputes.[108]

Institutionalization

[edit]
Military forces, such as the Polish Armed Forces seen here, are an example of an institution that handles conflict.

The institutionalization of conflicts refers to the resolution or settlement of a conflict when it has been passed on to an institution.[109] However, delegation to institutions brings about a lack of freedom in terms of the distribution structure of entitlements and offers. The parties to the conflict are directed by persons not involved in the conflict. In this process, the emotional and factual components of the conflict are separated. The institution or instance proceeds with rules that are mutually recognized by the parties to the conflict.

Feud

[edit]

Conflicts can be strongly or weakly institutionalized. The feud, a weakly institutionalized conflict, has some rules that are recognized by the parties to the conflict (e.g., existence of a legitimate feud reason, formal announcement, procedure, etc.), all signs of institutionalization, but on the other hand, the conflict is handled by the parties to the conflict themselves (no social differentiation); the emotional and factual component of the conflict are not separated: friends of the respective parties to the conflict not only have the right, but even the duty to assist, and they generate further conflicts.

Justice system

[edit]

The justice system is a national competitive system (interest of one party is to be served) for the regulation of conflicts. The procedures are divided into criminal procedures and civil procedures. The procedures are used when a legal claim is to be negotiated.

Military

[edit]

The military is used, among other things, in the event of a very strong escalation of a conflict between states or paramilitary groups. It is a competitive system with comparatively strong damages and collateral damages and is therefore used as a last resort. Early written works on military conflict resolution are The Art of War by Sunzi[110] and On War by Carl von Clausewitz.[111]

Divorce

[edit]

A divorce is a judicial conflict management system. If there are children, sometimes mediation, counseling or child protective services are used following a separation of parents.

Company conflicts

[edit]

Conflicts between employees of a company can create conflict cost to the company.[112][113] There is On-the-job training and Coaching for conflict management.

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q Donelson R. Forsyth: ‘‘Group Dynamics.’’ 7th edition. Cengage, 2018, ISBN 978-1-337-40885-1. Chapter 13: ‘‘Conflict.’’ P. 411–443.
  2. ^ a b c Glasl, F. (1982). The Process of Conflict Escalation and Roles of Third Parties. In: Bomers, G.B.J., Peterson, R.B. (eds) Conflict Management and Industrial Relations. Springer, Dordrecht.
  3. ^ Gerhard Schwarz: Konfliktmanagement. 9. Auflage. Gabler Verlag eBooks, 2014. doi:10.1007/978-3-8349-4598-3. ISBN 978-3-8349-4597-6, p. 36. Nach Pesendorfer, 2004.
  4. ^ M. Afzalur Rahim (2011), Managing Conflict in Organizations, Transaction Publishers, ISBN 978-1-4128-1456-0
  5. ^ Frank R. Pfetsch: Konflikt und Konfliktbewältigung. Beispiele für Formen zwischenstaatlicher Auseinandersetzungen. Tempora, 1994, ISBN 3-12-490400-1.
  6. ^ Michael Nicholson (1992). Rationality and the analysis of international conflict (in German). Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-39125-3. OCLC 23687612.
  7. ^ Robert D. Nye: Conflict among Humans. Springer, 1973, ISBN 0-8261-1371-0.
  8. ^ Stuart M. Schmidt, Thomas A. Kochan: Conflict: Toward Conceptual Clarity. In: Administrative Science Quarterly. 1972, Band 17, Nummer 3, S. 359 doi:10.2307/2392149.
  9. ^ Jessie Bernard: The Conceptualization of Intergroup Relations: With Special Reference to Conflict. In: Social Forces. 1951, Band 29, Nummer 3, S. 243–251 doi:10.2307/2572412.
  10. ^ Sidorenkov, Andrey V; Borokhovski, Evgueni F; Kovalenko, Viktor A (October 2018). "Group size and composition of work groups as precursors of intragroup conflicts". Psychology Research and Behavior Management. 11: 511–523. doi:10.2147/prbm.s178761. ISSN 1179-1578. PMC 6207408. PMID 30498379.
  11. ^ a b "Types of Group Conflict: Guide for Managers | High Speed Training". The Hub | High Speed Training. 2017-03-06. Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  12. ^ a b Jowett, Sophia; Lavallee, David (2007). Social psychology in sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. ISBN 978-0-7360-5780-6. OCLC 64770988.
  13. ^ a b c d Jehn, K. A.; Mannix, E. A. (2001-04-01). "The Dynamic Nature of Conflict: A Longitudinal Study of Intragroup Conflict and Group Performance". Academy of Management Journal. 44 (2): 238–251. ISSN 0001-4273. JSTOR 3069453.
  14. ^ Järvisalo, J.; Saris, N. E. (1975-09-15). "Action of propranolol on mitochondrial functions--effects on energized ion fluxes in the presence of valinomycin". Biochemical Pharmacology. 24 (18): 1701–1705. doi:10.1016/0006-2952(75)90009-x. ISSN 0006-2952. PMID 13.
  15. ^ a b Amason, Allen C.; Sapienza, Harry J. (August 1997). "The Effects of Top Management Team Size and interaction Norms on Cognitive and Affective Conflict". Journal of Management. 23 (4): 495–516. doi:10.1177/014920639702300401. ISSN 0149-2063. S2CID 143833786.
  16. ^ "Conflict Of Interest", Encyclopedia of Evaluation, Thousand Oaks California: Sage Publications, Inc., 2005, doi:10.4135/9781412950558.n97, ISBN 978-0-7619-2609-2
  17. ^ a b Lee, Yueh-Ting (2004). The psychology of ethnic and cultural conflict. Westport, Conn: Praeger. ISBN 9780275979836.
  18. ^ MacDonald, Kevin (2009-04-01). "Evolution, Psychology, and a Conflict Theory of Culture". Evolutionary Psychology. 7 (2): 147470490900700206. doi:10.1177/147470490900700206. ISSN 1474-7049. S2CID 4247168.
  19. ^ "Organizational conflict | definition in the Cambridge English Dictionary". dictionary.cambridge.org. Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  20. ^ "Definition of role conflict | Dictionary.com". www.dictionary.com. Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  21. ^ "What Is Work-Family Conflict? | Baylor Online". Baylor University MBA Online Degree Program. Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  22. ^ A. R. Overton, A. C. Lowry: Conflict management: difficult conversations with difficult people. In: Clinics in Colon and Rectal Surgery. Volume 26, Number 4, December 2013, pp. 259-264, doi:10.1055/s-0033-1356728. PMID 24436688, PMC 3835442.
  23. ^ Herbert Pietschmann: Vom Umgang mit wissenschaftlichen Konflikten. Movement on the waterfront. In: Würzburger medizinhistorische Mitteilungen. Volume 24, 2005, pp. 425-433.
  24. ^ Karl Berkel, Konfliktmanagement. In: Fritz Neske, Markus Wiener (Hrsg.): Management-Lexikon. Band II, 1985, S. 659 f.
  25. ^ a b c Richard Bösch: Conflict Escalation. In: Oxford Research Encyclopedia of International Studies. 2017 doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780190846626.013.82.
  26. ^ H. H. Saunders: Review of Preventing Violent Conflicts: A Strategy for Preventive Diplomacy; The Management of Conflict: Interpretations and Interests in Comparative Perspective, by M. S. Lund & M. H. Ross. In: Political Psychology. (1998), Band 19, Heft 4, S. 859–865.
  27. ^ M. S. Lund: Preventing violent conflicts: A strategy for preventive diplomacy. 1996. Washington, DC: United States Institute of Peace Press.
  28. ^ Ramsbotham, O., Woodhouse, T., & H. Miall: Contemporary conflict resolution. Polity, Cambridge, U.K. 2011.
  29. ^ a b Morton Deutsch, Peter T. Coleman, Eric C. Marcus: The handbook of conflict resolution : theory and practice. 2. Auflage. John Wiley, 2006, ISBN 0-7879-8058-7, S. 853–867.
  30. ^ Bösch, Richard (2017). "Conflict Escalation". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of International Studies. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780190846626.013.82. ISBN 978-0-19-084662-6.
  31. ^ Jürg Kuster, Eugen Huber, Robert Lippmann, Alphons Schmid, Emil Schneider, Urs Witschi, Roger Wüst (2008). Handbook of Project Management. Springer-Verlag. pp. 258 =. ISBN 978-3-540-76432-8.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  32. ^ a b Hans Fischer (2015). Learning to Lead Sustainably: The Comprehensive Leadership Concept PENTA for Sustainable Leadership Impact. vdf Hochschulverlag AG. p. 189. ISBN 978-3-7281-3667-1.
  33. ^ Norbert J. Heigl (2014). Understanding and Controlling Conflicts. Springer. p. 24. ISBN 978-3-658-04584-5.
  34. ^ Paul Graham (2008). "How to Disagree". Retrieved 2022-03-09.
  35. ^ Christian R. Dickmann: ‘‘Resolving Conflicts: What You Should Know as a Leader about Conflicts in the Corporate Environment’’, epubli, 2015, ISBN 978-3-7375-2373-8, p. 22.
  36. ^ Ronald Kraybill: ‘‘Style Matters: The Kraybill Conflict Style Inventory.’’ Riverhouse, 2005, ISBN 0-9755071-2-5.
  37. ^ Ernest H. O’Boyle, Donelson R. Forsyth: ‘‘Individual differences in ethics positions: The EPQ-5.’’ In: ‘‘PLOS ONE.’’ 2021, Volume 16, Number 6, p. e0251989 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0251989. PMID 34153037. PMC 8216522.
  38. ^ Robert Hogan, Joyce Hogan, Rodney Warrenfeltz (2007). The Hogan Guide. Hogan Assessment Systems. ISBN 978-0-9816457-6-6.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  39. ^ Ryan O. Murphy, Kurt A. Ackermann, Michel J. J. Handgraaf: ‘‘Measuring Social Value Orientation.’’ In: ‘‘Judgment and Decision Making.’’ 2011, Volume 6, Number 8, p. 771–781. doi:10.1017/S1930297500004204.
  40. ^ W. T. Au, J. Y. Y. Kwong: ‘‘Measurements and Effects of Social-Value Orientation in Social Dilemmas: A Review.’’ In: R. Suleiman, D. V. Budescu, I. Fischer, D. M. Messick (Eds.): ‘‘Contemporary psychological research on social dilemmas’’. Cambridge University, 2004, ISBN 0-521-80892-8, p. 71–98.
  41. ^ K. Lee, M. C. Ashton: ‘‘Psychometric Properties of the HEXACO Personality Inventory.’’ In: ‘‘Multivariate behavioral research.’’ Volume 39, Number 2, April 2004, p. 329–358, doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr3902_8. PMID 26804579.
  42. ^ C. J. Soto, O. P. John: ‘‘The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power.’’ In: ‘‘Journal of personality and social psychology.’’ Volume 113, Number 1, July 2017, p. 117–143, doi:10.1037/pspp0000096. PMID 27055049.
  43. ^ L. Ross, A. Ward: ‘‘Naïve Realism in Everyday Life: Implications of Social Conflict and Misunderstanding.’’ In: Edward S. Reed, Elliot Turiel, Terrance Brown (Eds.): ‘‘Values and Knowledge.’’ In: ‘‘Psychology Press eBooks.’’ 2013, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. doi:10.4324/9780203773772, p. 103–135.
  44. ^ David C. Wilson: ‘‘A Strategy of Change: Concepts and Controversies in the Management of Change’’. ITBP Textbooks Series, Routledge series in analytical management. Thomson, 1992, ISBN 1-86152-383-1.
  45. ^ George A. Youngs: ‘‘Patterns of threat and punishment reciprocity in a conflict setting.’’ In: ‘‘Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.’’ 1986, Volume 51, Number 3, p. 541–546. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.541.
  46. ^ Morton Deutsch, Peter T. Coleman, Eric C. Marcus: The handbook of conflict resolution : theory and practice. 2. Auflage. John Wiley, 2006, ISBN 0-7879-8058-7, S. XIV.
  47. ^ C. K. W. De Dreu, B. Beersma, W. Steinel, G. A. Van Kleef: ‘‘The psychology of negotiation: Principles and basic processes.’’ In: A. W. Kruglanski, E. T. Higgins (Eds.): ‘‘Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles.’’ 2nd edition. The Guilford Press, 2007, ISBN 978-1-4625-5024-1, p. 608–629.
  48. ^ Gerben A. van Kleef, Carsten K. W. De Dreu, Antony Stephen Reid Manstead: ‘‘The Interpersonal Effects of Emotions in Negotiations: A Motivated Information Processing Approach.’’ In: ‘‘Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.’’ 2004, Volume 87, Number 4, p. 510–528. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.510.
  49. ^ a b c d e f Morton Deutsch, Peter T. Coleman, Eric C. Marcus: ‘‘The handbook of conflict resolution : theory and practice.’’ 2nd edition. John Wiley, 2006, ISBN 0-7879-8058-7, p. 351.
  50. ^ L. Thompson, J. Nadler: ‘Judgmental biases in conflict resolution and how to overcome them.’ In: M. Deutsch, P. Coleman (Eds.): ‘Handbook of constructive conflict resolution: theory and practice’. Jossey-Bass/Wiley, 2000, ISBN 0-7879-4822-5, pp. 213–235.
  51. ^ L. Ross: ‘The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process.’ In: L. Berkowitz: ‘Advances in experimental social psychology.’ Volume 10. Academic Press. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3.
  52. ^ L. A. Borah: ‘The effects of threat in bargaining: Critical and experimental analysis.’ In: ‘The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology.’ 1963, Volume 66, Number 1, pp. 37–44. doi:10.1037/h0043937.
  53. ^ Philip S. Gallo: ‘Effects of increased incentives upon the use of threat in bargaining.’ In: ‘Journal of Personality and Social Psychology.’ 1966, Volume 4, Number 1, pp. 14–20. doi:10.1037/h0023510.
  54. ^ Jared R. Curhan, Margaret A. Neale, Lee Ross: ‘Dynamic valuation: Preference changes in the context of face-to-face negotiation.’ In: ‘Journal of Experimental Social Psychology.’ 2004, Volume 40, Number 2, pp. 142–151. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.12.002.
  55. ^ Sharon S. Brehm and Jack W. Brehm: ‘Psychological Reactance.’ Elsevier eBooks, 1981. doi:10.1016/C2013-0-10423-0.
  56. ^ Edward J. Lawler, Rebecca Ford, Mary A. Blegen: ‘Coercive Capability in Conflict: A Test of Bilateral Deterrence Versus Conflict Spiral Theory.’ In: ‘Social Psychology Quarterly.’ 1988, Volume 51, Number 2, pp. 93. doi:10.2307/2786833.
  57. ^ Tamar Saguy, Michal Reifen‐Tagar: ‘‘The social psychological roots of violent intergroup conflict.’’ In: ‘‘Nature Reviews Psychology.’’ 2022, Volume 1, Number 10, pp. 577–589. doi:10.1038/s44159-022-00083-7.
  58. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Donelson R. Forsyth: ‘‘Group Dynamics.’’ 7th Edition. Cengage, 2018, ISBN 978-1-337-40885-1. Chapter 14: ‘‘Intergroup Relations.’’ pp. 444–478.
  59. ^ R. J. Eidelson, J. I. Eidelson: ‘‘Dangerous ideas. Five beliefs that propel groups toward conflict.’’ In: ‘‘The American Psychologist.’’ Volume 58, Number 3, March 2003, pp. 182–192, doi:10.1037/0003-066x.58.3.182. PMID 12772423.
  60. ^ a b c d Amy J. C. Cuddy, Susan T. Fiske, Peter Glick: The BIAS map: Behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. In: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007, volume 92, issue 4, p. 631–648 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.631.
  61. ^ a b c d Susan T. Fiske, Amy J. C. Cuddy, Peter Glick: Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence. In: Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006, volume 11, issue 2, p. 77–83 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005.
  62. ^ Morton Deutsch, Peter T. Coleman, Eric C. Marcus: ‘‘The handbook of conflict resolution : theory and practice.’’ 2nd Edition. John Wiley, 2006, ISBN 0-7879-8058-7, p. 185.
  63. ^ Jim Sidanius, Hillary Haley, Ludwin E. Molina, Felicia Pratto: ‘‘Vladimir's Choice and the Distribution of Social Resources.’’ In: ‘‘Group Processes & Intergroup Relations.’’ 2007, Volume 10, Number 2, pp. 257–265. doi:10.1177/1368430207074732.
  64. ^ Fischer, Michael D. (September 2012). "Organizational Turbulence, Trouble and Trauma: Theorizing the Collapse of a Mental Health Setting". Organization Studies. 33 (9): 1153–1173. doi:10.1177/0170840612448155. ISSN 0170-8406. S2CID 52219788.
  65. ^ Fischer, Michael Daniel; Ferlie, Ewan (January 2013). "Resisting hybridisation between modes of clinical risk management: Contradiction, contest, and the production of intractable conflict". Accounting, Organizations and Society. 38 (1): 30–49. doi:10.1016/j.aos.2012.11.002. S2CID 44146410.
  66. ^ Bösch, Richard (2017-11-20). "Conflict Escalation". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of International Studies. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of International Studies. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780190846626.013.82. ISBN 978-0-19-084662-6. Retrieved 2020-07-18.
  67. ^ Forsyth, D.R. (2010). Group Dynamics (5th Edition). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
  68. ^ Ross, L., & Ward, A. (1995). Naïve Realism in Everyday Life: Implications of Social Conflict and Misunderstanding. In T. Brown, E.S. Reed, & E. Turiel (Eds.), Values and Knowledge (pp. 103–135). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  69. ^ Wilson, D. C. (1992). A Strategy of Change. London: Routledge.
  70. ^ Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (1981). Psychological Reactance: A Theory of Freedom and Control. Academic Press.
  71. ^ Curhan, J.R., Neale, M.A., & Ross, L. (2004). Dynamic valuation: Preference changes in the context of face-to-face negotiations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 142–151.
  72. ^ Thompson, L., & Nadler, J. (2000). Judgmental biases in conflict resolution and how to overcome them. In M. Deutsch, & P. Coleman (Eds.), Handbook of constructive conflict resolution: theory and practice (pp. 213–235).
  73. ^ Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 10). New York: Academic Press.
  74. ^ Bar-Tal, D. (2007). Living with the conflict: Socio-psychological analysis of the Israeli-Jewish society. Jerusalem: Carmel. (in Hebrew).
  75. ^ Sattler, D. N., & Kerr, N. L. (1991). Might versus morality explored: Motivational and cognitive bases for social motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 756–765.
  76. ^ Van Kleef, G. A., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2002). Social value orientation and impression formation: A test of two competing hypotheses about information search in negotiation. International Journal of Conflict Management, 13, 59–77.
  77. ^ Steinel, W., and De Dreu, C. K. W. (2004). Social motives and strategic misrepresentation in social decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 419–434.
  78. ^ Mikolic, J. M., Parker, J. C., & Pruitt, D. G. (1997). Escalation in response to persistent annoyance: Groups versus individuals and gender effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 151–163.
  79. ^ Deutsch, M., & Krauss, R.M. (1960). The effect of threat upon interpersonal bargaining. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61, 181–189.
  80. ^ Krauss, R. M., & Morsella, E. (2000/2007). Communication and conflict. In M. Deutsch & P. T. Coleman (Eds.), The Handbook of Conflict Resolution: Theory and Practice (pp. 131–143). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
  81. ^ Borah, L.A., Jr. (1963). The effects of threat in bargaining: Critical and experimental analysis. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 37–44.
  82. ^ Gallo, P. S. (1966). Effects of increased incentives upon the use of threat in bargaining. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 14–20.
  83. ^ Lawler, E. J., Ford, R. S., & Blegen, M. A. (1988). Coercive capability in conflict: A test of bilateral deterrence versus conflict spiral theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 51(2), 93–107.
  84. ^ Youngs, G. A., Jr. (1986). Patterns of threat and punishment reciprocity in a conflict setting. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 51, 541–546.
  85. ^ Mannix, E. A. (1993). Organizations as resource dilemmas: The effects of power balance on coalition formation in small groups. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 55, 1–22.
  86. ^ De Dreu, C. K. W., Beersma, B., Steinel, W., & Van Kleef, G. A. (2007). The psychology of negotiation: Principles and basic processes. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (2nd ed. pp. 608–629). New York: Guilford.
  87. ^ Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2004). The interpersonal effects of emotions in negotiations: A motivated information processing approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 510–528.
  88. ^ Michael Nicholson (27 March 1992). Rationality and the Analysis of International Conflict. Cambridge University Press. p. 13. ISBN 978-0-521-39810-7. Retrieved 11 October 2012.
  89. ^ Sophia Jowett (2007). Social Psychology in Sport. Human Kinetics. p. 34. ISBN 978-0-7360-5780-6. Retrieved 11 October 2012.
  90. ^ Morton Deutsch, Peter T. Coleman, Eric C. Marcus: ‘‘The handbook of conflict resolution : theory and practice.’’ 2nd Edition. John Wiley, 2006, ISBN 0-7879-8058-7, p. 17.
  91. ^ Ines Lietzke: ‘‘Conflict resolution through Europeanization. The negotiations on the status of Kosovo.’’ Springer Fachmedien, 2015, ISBN 978-3-658-10956-1, p. 46.
  92. ^ Roger Fisher, William Ury, Bruce M. Patton (Eds.): ‘‘The Harvard Concept. The Classic of Negotiation Technique’’. 1984; 24th edition, Campus, 2013, ISBN 978-3-593-39920-1.
  93. ^ Josef W. Seifert: Visualize – Present – Moderate, 30th edition, Gabal Verlag, Offenbach 2011, ISBN 978-3-86936-240-3.
  94. ^ Thomas Gordon: ‘‘Parent Effectiveness Training. The Proven Program for Raising Responsible Children’’. Three Rivers Press, 2000, ISBN 0-609-80693-9, p. 153.
  95. ^ Thomas Gordon: ‘‘Family Conference in Practice: How Conflicts with Children are Resolved’’. Heyne Verlag, 2012, ISBN 978-3-641-07172-1, p. 116.
  96. ^ Marshall B. Rosenberg: ‘‘Nonviolent Communication – A Language of Life’’. 3rd edition. PuddleDancer Press, 2015, ISBN 978-1-892005-28-1.
  97. ^ Friedemann Schulz von Thun: ‘’Talking to Each Other.‘’ Volume 1: ‘‘Disturbances and Clarifications. Psychology of interpersonal communication’’. Rowohlt, Reinbek 1981, ISBN 3-499-17489-8.
  98. ^ Thomas L. Ruble, Kenneth W. Thomas, ‘‘Support for a two-dimensional model of conflict behavior.’’ Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, Ch. 16, 1976, p. 145; Rosemary Thomson and Eion Farmer, ‘‘Managing Relationships’’, Open University, Milton Keynes, 1999, ISBN 0-7492-9548-1, pp. 36/37.
  99. ^ Thomas L. Ruble, Richard A. Cosier (1980-03-13). An Exercise in Conflict-Handling Behavior. Developments in Business Simulation and Experiential Learning. Vol. 7.
  100. ^ David A. Whetten, Kim S. Cameron: ‘‘Contract re-design.’’ In: ‘‘Personnel Administrator.’’ 34, No. 10, 1988, pp. 97–101.
  101. ^ Jane Srygley Mouton, Robert Rogers Blake: ‘‘The Managerial Grid: The Key to Leadership Excellence’’. Gulf Publishing Co., 1964, ISBN 0-88415-252-9.
  102. ^ Kenneth Wayne Thomas, Ralph H. Kilmann: ‘‘Thomas-Kilmann Conflict Mode Instrument’’. XICOM, 1974.
  103. ^ Kenneth W. Thomas, Ralph H. Kilmann: ‘‘Comparison of Four Instruments Measuring Conflict Behavior.’’ In: ‘‘Psychological Reports.’’ 1978, Volume 42, Number 3_suppl, pp. 1139–1145 doi:10.2466/pr0.1978.42.3c.1139.
  104. ^ Werner Fleischer, Benedikt Fleischer, Martin Monninger: ‘‘Role and behavior profiles: Resolving conflicts constructively. Volume 4.’’ Kohlhammer, 2022, ISBN 978-3-17-035778-5, pp. 114–121.
  105. ^ Paul Anker: ‘‘Mastering Conflicts. Handbook for Scrum Masters in Agile Teams.’’ Tredition, 2023, ISBN 978-3-347-98519-3.
  106. ^ University of Cambridge Judge Business School: ‘‘Understanding conflict: The Thomas‐Kilmann Conflict Mode Instrument’’ (PDF; 0.4 MB), retrieved on February 11, 2024.
  107. ^ a b Forsyth, 2010.
  108. ^ http://jce.sagepub.com/content/40/1/39.short Wästerfors, David (2011) "Disputes and Going Concerns" Journal of Contemporary Ethnography (40) 1: 39–70
  109. ^ Ralf Dahrendorf: Pfade aus Utopia. München 1974. ISBN 978-3-4921-0101-1.
  110. ^ Lionel Giles: The Art of War. Translation Chinese - English, 1910, at the Internet Archive.
  111. ^ Carl von Clausewitz: Vom Kriege., Bd. 1–3, Ferdinand Dümmler, Berlin 1832–1834 (Edited by Marie von Clausewitz), (online)
  112. ^ V. Gaba, J. Joseph: Content and process: organizational conflict and decision making. In: Frontiers in psychology. Band 14, 2023, S. 1227966, doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1227966. PMID 38034290, PMC 1068196.
  113. ^ Chinyere N. Alimba: Conflict Management Styles: Historical evolution, perspectives and rationalisation. (PDF; 1,1 MB). In: Review of Public Administration and Management. (2018), Band 7, Heft 1, S. 24–36.